Relationship between stuttering severity in children and their mothers’ speaking rate (2024)

  • Journal List
  • Sao Paulo Med J
  • v.126(1); 2008
  • PMC11020510

As a library, NLM provides access to scientific literature. Inclusion in an NLM database does not imply endorsem*nt of, or agreement with, the contents by NLM or the National Institutes of Health.
Learn more: PMC Disclaimer | PMC Copyright Notice

Relationship between stuttering severity in children and their mothers’ speaking rate (1)

Link to Publisher's site

Sao Paulo Med J. 2008; 126(1): 29–33.

Published online 2008 Jan 3. doi:10.1590/S1516-31802008000100006

PMCID: PMC11020510

PMID: 18425284

Language: English | Portuguese

Ali Dehqan,Relationship between stuttering severity in children and their mothers’ speaking rate (2)* Mehdi Bakhtiar,* Sadegh Seif Panahi,* and Hassan Ashayeri*

Author information Article notes Copyright and License information PMC Disclaimer

ABSTRACT

CONTEXT AND OBJECTIVE:

Stuttering is a complex disease that influences occupational, social, academic and emotional achievements. The aim of this study was to correlate the stuttering severity index with speaking rates of mothers and children.

DESIGN AND SETTING:

Cross-sectional study, at the child rehabilitation clinics of Tehran city.

METHODS:

35 pairs of mothers and their children who stuttered were studied. There were 29 boys and six girls, of mean age 8.5 years (range: 5.1-12.0). Speech samples from the mother-child pairs were audiotaped for approximately 15 minutes, until a reciprocal verbal interaction had been obtained. This sample was then analyzed in accordance with a stuttering severity index test and speaking rate parameters.

RESULTS:

The research results outlined a significant relationship between the mothers’ speaking rate and their children's stuttering severity.

CONCLUSION:

The results suggest that the mothers’ speaking rate should be incorporated in the assessment and treatment of stuttering.

KEY WORDS: Speech, Language, Communication, Stuttering, Mother(s)

RESUMO

CONTEXTO E OBJETIVO:

Gagueira é uma doença complexa que tem influência nas realizações ocupacionais, sociais, acadêmicas e emocionais. A finalidade deste trabalho foi correlacionar o índice de gravidade da gagueira de crianças com as taxas de velocidade de fala das mães.

TIPO DE ESTUDO E LOCAL:

Estudo transversal, realizado na clínica infantil de reabilitação da cidade de Teerã.

MÉTODOS:

Uma amostra da fala de 35 crianças gagas pareadas com suas mães, sendo 29 meninos e 6 meninas, de 5:1-12:0 anos (idade média de 8.5), foi gravada por aproximadamente 15 minutos para avaliar a interação verbal recíproca, sendo então analisada de acordo com o índice de gravidade da gagueira e com os parâmetros de taxa de velocidade de fala.

RESULTADOS:

Os resultados da pesquisa mostraram uma relação significante entre a taxa de velocidade de fala da mãe e a gravidade da gagueira da criança.

PALAVRAS-CHAVE: Fala, Linguagem, Comunicação, Gagueira, Mães

INTRODUCTION

Stuttering is described as a disorder of fluency and is characterized by part-word, whole-word and phrase repetitions, interjections, pauses and prolongations.1 Perhaps no speech problem has received more attention than stuttering. A wide variety of theories have been proposed based on the enormous volume of research findings. Some theories have proposed physiological factors for the onset of stuttering, such as bilateral hemispheric dominance,2 right hemispheric dominance for speech,2,3 brain damage,4,5 neuropsychological or neuromuscular dysfunction,6 laryngeal dysfunction7 and central auditory dysfunction.8 Data on the frequency of stuttering among relatives of those who stutter have led some investigators to propose a genetic component to stuttering.913 Others have suggested environmental factors for both the onset and the maintenance of stuttering, such as communicative stress,14 anxiety,15 personality and negative parent-child interactions.1618

Although the recent advances in imaging techniques have shifted attention to neurological and/or physiological factors for the onset or cause of stuttering,19,20 the communication environment that stutterers live in may contribute towards maintaining the stuttering. Moreover, this communication environment that stuttering children live in may even play an important role in the success or failure of speech therapy. In other words, the role of the environment and, in particular, the linguistic and paralinguistic behavior and attitudes of parents have frequently been cited in both theoretical and clinical literature as presenting important correlations with the onset and development of stuttering among young children.2125

Clinical intervention strategies currently used for children who stutter also frequently focus on the parents’ role, instructing them to alter their linguistic behavior (e.g. by reducing negative statements regarding their child's speech and/or stuttering) and their paralinguistic behavior (e.g. by reducing their overall speech rate).24,2637 For example, Guitar and Marchinkoski38 and others39 reported that reductions in mothers’ speaking rates resulted in similar reductions in children's speaking rates and corresponding improvements in speech fluency for some children who stuttered.

Past studies have observed parents from a unidirectional perspective. Research questions have centered on the idea that the parents of stutterers were different from the parents of nonstutterers. To conduct bidirectional research, Meyers and Freeman40 explored the notion that the parents of children who stutter are “habitually fast talkers” and reported that the mothers of children who stutter spoke significantly faster than the mothers of nonstutterers did. Based on samples of the 15 longest perceptibly fluent utterances produced by each child, Meyers and Freeman40 also found that the stuttering children spoke significantly more slowly during their fluent speech than did their nonstuttering peers, and that the children with severe stuttering talked more slowly than did the children with moderately severe stuttering.40

It has been hypothesized that alterations in parental speaking rates may influence the speaking rates of children who stutter.21,22,40,41 Guitar and Marchinkoski38 investigated the effects on children's speech rate when their mothers talked more slowly and reported that when mothers substantially decreased their speech rates in a controlled situation, their children also decreased their speech rates.38

OBJECTIVE

The aim of this study was to correlate the stuttering severity index with the speaking rates of mothers and their children who stutter.

METHODS

Subjects: 35 pairs of mothers and their children who stuttered took part in this study. There were 29 boys and 6 girls, and their mean age was 8.5 years (range 5.1-12.0 years). The participants in this study were selected from the child rehabilitation clinics of Tehran University of Medical Sciences, in the city of Tehran. The selection criterion for the mothers of these children was that they should have normal speech, as diagnosed by a speech-language pathologist. The criterion for the children was that none of them had been evaluated for any fluency disorder or had received stuttering therapy prior to this study. Children who exhibited any clinically significant hearing, articulation, voice, language, psychological/emotional or physical problems that were considered atypical for their chronological age and level of development were excluded from this study.

Following data collection, a further assessment of stuttering was conducted by two certified speech-language pathologists. Children were classified as stutterers if they exhibited three or more within-word dysfluencies (i.e. sound prolongations, sound/syllable repetitions, monosyllabic whole-word repetitions or broken words) per 100 words during a 300-word sample of audiotaped conversation with their mothers, and if people in their environments had expressed concern regarding their speech fluency.

Procedure: Each child and his/her mother were seated facing each other at a small table on which a series of action pictures had been placed. They were instructed to talk about these action pictures. Each mother-child pair was audiotaped for approximately 15 minutes, or until a sufficient sample from the child was obtained. All mother-child verbal interactions were audiotaped.

Following the recording session, the utterances produced by each mother-child pair, i.e. the “unit(s) of language preceded and followed by a perceived pause or terminated by some change in inflection (rising or falling intonation)”,42 were orthographically transcribed by the first author. A 10-minute warm-up period of audiotaping was conducted prior to data collection, during which the subjects had the opportunity to become familiar with the equipment and materials present in the test environment. The number of syllables per utterance spoken by the mother and child was included on the transcript. The location and type of each within-word and between-word (i.e. multisyllabic whole-word repetition, phrase repetition, revision or interjection) speech dysfluency produced by each mother and child was also indicated. Finally, two examiners checked the transcriptions.

The speaking rate in the present study was equivalent to articulatory rate, which is defined as a number of syllables (AR-S) produced per minute of nonstuttered speech.41,43

The stuttering severity was determined by means of an international instrument (the Stuttering Severity instrument). This test assesses the frequency and duration of speech disruptions, and also the presence of physical concomitants associated with these disruptions.44 The children who stuttered were divided into three groups based on their stuttering severity: 12 subjects were in the mild group, 15 in the moderate group and eight in the severe group.

Data analysis: One-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) was performed to compare the mothers’ speaking rates and their children's stuttering severity and also to compare the children's speaking rates and their stuttering severity. The Pearson correlation was used to compare the speaking rates of the mothers and their stuttering children. The SPSS software (version 12) was used for data analysis.

RESULTS

As illustrated in Table 1 and Figure 1, there was a significant relationship between the means for the mothers’ speaking rates and their children's stuttering severity. In other words, faster speaking rates among mothers were associated with greater stuttering severity in their children (p < 0.01).

Table 1.

Comparison between mothers’ speaking rates and children's stuttering severity

Children's stuttering severityMothers’ speaking rate95% CIFp-value
MeanSDLower limitUpper limit
Mild
(n = 12)
242.302.96240.42244.19
Moderate
(n = 15)
252.082.32250.80253.37137.97< 0.01*
Severe
(n = 8)
264.183.68261.10267.26

Open in a separate window

*statistically significant; CI = confidence interval; SD = Standard Deviation.

Open in a separate window

Figure 1

Severity index and syllables per minute among mothers.

On the other hand, as shown in Table 2, there was a significant negative correlation between the means for the mothers’ speaking rates and their children's speaking rates (p < 0.01). Thus, the faster the mother's speaking rate was, the slower her child's speaking rate was.

Table 2.

Comparison between speaking rates of mothers and children

Speaking ratePearson correlationp-value
MeanSD
Mother251.498.70- 0.92< 0.01*
Child115.5017.70

Open in a separate window

*statistically significant; SD = standard deviation.

Finally, Table 3 and Figure 2 present an intragroup comparison between the children's speaking rates and their stuttering severity. This showed a significant correlation (p < 0.01) such that increased stuttering severity was associated with decreased speaking rates among the children who stuttered. In other words, the children with severe stuttering severity had a slower speaking rate than the moderate group did, and the moderate group had a slower speaking rate than the mild group did.

Table 3.

Comparison between children's speaking rates and their stuttering severity

Children's stuttering severityChildren's speaking rates95% CIFp-value
MeanSDLower limitUpper limit
Mild
(n = 12)
134.455.07131.22137.67
Moderate
(n = 15)
113.667.55109.48117.85137.97< 0.01*
Severe
(n = 8)
90.545.7285.7595.33

Open in a separate window

*statistically significant; CI = confidence interval; SD = standard deviation.

Open in a separate window

Figure 2

Severity index and syllables per minute among the children who stuttered.

DISCUSSION

Despite the behavioral complexity of a stuttering problem, dysfluency often plays a primary role in differential diagnostic decisions and treatment evaluations. It is known that absolute continuity of speech production is physiologically impossible. A perception of continuous speech can be obtained from the number of audible speech utterances and the shortness of the physiological pauses (e.g. intervals for swallowing and breathing), and from the linguistic pauses (e.g. memory effects and lexical access) that are pertinent and expected from any speaker.45 The present study regarded the mother's speaking model as an important part of her child's interaction environment, which had an impact on the child's speaking model and was associated with the severity of the problem. This basic result held true in the present study, such that with increased speaking rates among the mothers with stuttering children, their children's stuttering severity would also be increased.

As was noted in the present study, the mothers with high speaking rates imposed more time pressure and communication stress on their small conversation partners. Thus, their children felt under more stress, which would result in enhancement of their stuttering severity.

On the other hand, enhancement of the children's stuttering severity would lead their mothers to get into a “nervous state” and they would compensate for this by increasing their speaking rates, in the hope that their children might increase their speaking rates.

Another result obtained from the present study was that, with increasing stuttering severity among these children, their speaking rate decreased. This result is also in line with the findings of Meyers and Freeman.40 Furthermore, the results from de Andrade et al.45 and from the present study have confirmed the findings previously published regarding speakers of American English, thereby pointing towards a direct relationship between increases in the stuttering severity index and reductions in speech rate.45

The latter result, showing that there is an interesting negative correlation between mother's and children's speaking rates, is in line with the findings of Ainsworth and Fraser,21 Conture and Fraser,22 Costello41 and Meyers and Freeman.40 All of these other studies hypothesized that alterations in parental speaking rates influenced the speaking rates of stuttering children. Moreover, the present study was in line with Meyers and Freeman,40 in concluding that the more the child stuttered, the slower he talked, and the slower the child talked, the faster the mother interacting with him talked. However, it is equally possible to interpret this analysis as demonstrating that the faster a mother spoke, the more the child stuttered, and the more he stuttered the slower he talked, and so forth.

There are several possible reasons why mothers might use a faster speaking rate when talking to a slow-talking or stuttering child. First, stuttering behavior may alter dialogue patterns. That is, a slow-speaking and/or stuttering child may disrupt the pace of the ongoing interaction, thus prompting the mother to speed her rate in the hope of increasing the child's rate. By talking faster, a mother may press the child to talk faster, and talking faster may lead to increased stuttering. Alternatively, a child's struggle for fluency, or his frequent dysfluency, may create internal discomfort, tension, anxiety or “nervousness” in his mother. Such internal reactions may alter a mother's speech motor behavior, thereby causing her to speak faster.40

CONCLUSION

The findings from the present study suggest that the speech rates of mothers and their children who stutter are important indicators of fluency levels among stuttering children and should be incorporated in the assessment and treatment of stuttering. Mothers, as their children's main communication partners, have an important role in the therapy process and in setting clinical strategies.

Biographies

Ali Dehqan, MSc. Lecturer, Department of Speech Therapy, School of Paramedicine, Zahedan University of Medical Sciences, Zahedan, Iran.

Mehdi Bakhtiar, MSc. Lecturer, Department of Speech Therapy, School of Paramedicine, Zahedan University of Medical Sciences, Zahedan, Iran.

Sadegh Seif Panahi, MSc. Lecturer, Department of Speech Therapy, School of Paramedicine, Zahedan University of Medical Sciences, Zahedan, Iran.

Hassan Ashayeri, MD. Professor, Department of Neurosciences, School of Rehabilitation, Iran University of Medical Sciences, Tehran, Iran.

Footnotes

Sources of funding: None

Zahedan University of Medical Sciences, Zahedan, Iran

REFERENCES

1. Hegde MN. Introduction to communicative disorders. 2nd ed. Texas: Pro-ed; 1995. [Google Scholar]

2. Curry FK, Gregory HH. The performance of stutterers on dichotic listening tasks thought to reflect cerebral dominance. J Speech Hear Res. 1969;12(1):73–82. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

3. Moore WH, Jr, Haynes WO. Alpha hemispheric asymmetry and stuttering: some support for a segmentation dysfunction hypothesis. J Speech Hear Res. 1980;23(2):229–247. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

4. Andrews G, Craig A, Feyer AM, Hoddinott S, Howie P, Neilson M. Stuttering: a review of research findings and theories circa 1982. J Speech Hear Disord. 1983;48(3):226–246. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

5. Shames GH, Ramig PR. Stuttering and other disorders of fluency. In: Shames GH, Wiig EH, Secord WA, editors. Human communication disorders: an introduction. 4th ed. New York: Prentice Hall College Div; 1994. pp. 336–387. [Google Scholar]

6. Van Riper C. Final thoughts about stuttering. Journal of Fluency Disorders. 1990;15(5-6):317–318. Available from: http://cat.inist.fr/?aModele=afficheN&cpsidt=5036205. Accessed in 2007 (Nov 9) [Google Scholar]

7. Reich A, Till J, Goldsmith H. Laryngeal and manual reaction times of stuttering and nonstuttering adults. J Speech Hear Res. 1981;24(2):192–196. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

8. Hall JW, Jerger J. Central auditory function in stutterers. J Speech Hear Res. 1978;21(2):324–337. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

9. Ambrose NG, Yairi E, Cox N. Genetic aspects of early childhood stuttering. J Speech Hear Res. 1993;36(4):701–706. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

10. Howie PM. Concordance for stuttering in monozygotic and dizygotic twin pairs. J Speech Hear Res. 1981;24(3):317–321. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

11. Poulos MG, Webster WG. Family history as a basis for subgrouping people who stutter. J Speech Hear Res. 1991;34(1):5–10. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

12. Yairi E. The onset of stuttering in two- and three-year-old children: a preliminary report. J Speech Hear Disord. 1983;48(2):171–177. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

13. Yairi E, Ambrose N. Onset of stuttering in preschool children: selected factors. J Speech Hear Res. 1992;35(4):782–788. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

14. Schulze H, Johannsen HS. Importance of parent-child interaction in the genesis of stuttering. Folia Phoniatr (Basel) 1991;43(3):133–143. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

15. Craig A. An investigation into the relationship between anxiety and stuttering. J Speech Hear Disord. 1990;55(2):290–294. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

16. Egolf DB, Shames GH, Johnson PR, Kasprisin-Burelli A. The use of parent-child interaction patterns in therapy for young stutterers. J Speech Hear Disord. 1972;37(2):222–232. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

17. Peters HFM, Starkweather CW. Development of stuttering throughout life. Jounal of Fluency Disorders. 1989;14(5):303–321. Available from: http://cat.inist.fr/?aModele=afficheN&cpsidt=6714999. Accessed in 2007 (Nov 9) [Google Scholar]

18. Meyers SC. Verbal behaviors of preschool stutterers and conversational partners: observing reciprocal relationships. J Speech Hear Disord. 1990;55(4):706–712. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

19. Wu JC, Maguire G, Riley G, et al. A positron emission tomography [18F]deoxyglucose study of developmental stuttering. Neuroreport. 1995;6(3):501–505. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

20. De Nil LF, Kroll RM, Kapur S, Houle S. A positron emission tomography study of silent and oral single word reading in stuttering and nonstuttering adults. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2000;43(4):1038–1053. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

21. Ainsworth SH, Fraser JH. If your child stutters: a guide for parents. 3rd ed. Memphis: Speech Foundation of America; 1988. [Google Scholar]

22. Conture EG, Fraser JH. Stuttering and your child: questions and answers. Memphis: Speech Foundation of America; 1989. [Google Scholar]

23. Luper HL, Mulder RL. Stuttering therapy for children. Englewood Cliffs: Prentice Hall; 1964. [Google Scholar]

24. Peters TJ, Guitar B. Stuttering: an integrated approach to its nature and treatment. Baltimore: Williams & Wilkins; 1991. [Google Scholar]

25. Van Riper C. The nature of stuttering. 2nd ed. Englewood Cliffs: Prentice-Hall; 1982. [Google Scholar]

26. Andrews G, Nellson M. Meeting of the American Speech Language Hearing Association. Los Angeles; 1981. Stuttering: a state-of-the-art seminar. [abstract] [Google Scholar]

27. Bloodstein O. Stuttering as tension and fragmentation. In: Eisenson J, editor. Stuttering: a second symposium. New York: Harper and Row; 1975. pp. 1–95. [Google Scholar]

28. Bloodstein O. A handbook on stuttering. 4th ed. Chicago: National Easter Seal Society; 1987. [Google Scholar]

29. Conture EG. Stuttering. 1st ed. Englewood Cliffs: Prentice-Hall; 1982. [Google Scholar]

30. Conture EG. Youngsters who stutter: diagnosis, parent counseling, and referral. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 1982;3(3):163–169. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

31. Conture EG. Stuttering. 2nd ed. Englewood Cliffs: Prentice-Hall; 1990. [Google Scholar]

32. Conture EG, Caruso AJ. Assessment and diagnosis of childhood dysfluency. In: Rowley D, Purser H, editors. Progress in the treatment of fluency disorders. London: Taylor & Francis; 1987. pp. 84–104. [Google Scholar]

33. Curlee RF, Perkins WH. Conversational rate control therapy for stuttering. J Speech Hear Disord. 1969;34(3):245–250. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

34. Shames GH, Florence CL. Stutter-free speech: a goal for therapy. Columbus Ohio: Charles E. Merrill; 1980. [Google Scholar]

35. Starkweather CW. Fluency and stuttering. Englewood Cliffs: Prentice-Hall; 1987. [Google Scholar]

36. Wall MJ, Myers FL. Clinical management of childhood stuttering. Baltimore: University Park Press; 1984. [Google Scholar]

37. Zebrowski PM, Conture EG. Judgments of disfluency by mothers of stuttering and normally fluent children. J Speech Hear Res. 1989;32(3):625–634. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

38. Guitar B, Marchinkoski L. Influence of mothers’ slower speech on their children's speech rate. J Speech Lang Hear Re. 2001;44(4):853–861. [Google Scholar]

39. Yaruss JS, Conture EG. Mother and child speaking rates and utterance lengths in adjacent fluent utterances: preliminary observations. Journal of Fluency Disorders. 1995;20(3):257–278. Available from: http://www.ingentaconnect.com/content/els/0094730x/1995/00000020/00000003/art00013. Accessed in 2007 (Nov 9) [Google Scholar]

40. Meyers SC, Freeman FJ. Mother and child speech rates as a variable in stuttering and disfluency. J Speech Hear Res. 1985;28(3):436–444. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

41. Costello JM. Current behavioral treatments for children. In: Prins D, Ingham RJ, editors. Treatment of stuttering in early childhood. San Diego: College-Hill; 1983. pp. 69–112. [Google Scholar]

42. Engler LF, Hannah EP, Longhurst TM. Linguistic analysis of speech samples: a practical guide for clinicians. J Speech Hear Disord. 1973;38(2):192–204. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

43. Pindzola RH, Jenkins MM, Lokken KJ. Speaking rates of young children. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools. 1989;20:133–138. Available from: http://lshss.asha.org/cgi/content/abstract/20/2/133. Accessed in 2007 (Nov 9) [Google Scholar]

44. Riley GD. A stuttering severity instrument for children and adults. J Speech Hear Disord. 1972;37(3):314–322. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

45. de Andrade CR, Cervone LM, Sassi FC. Relationship between the stuttering severity index and speech rate. Sao Paulo Med J. 2003;121(2):81–84. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from São Paulo Medical Journal are provided here courtesy of Associação Paulista de Medicina

Relationship between stuttering severity in children and their mothers’ speaking rate (2024)
Top Articles
Latest Posts
Article information

Author: Clemencia Bogisich Ret

Last Updated:

Views: 5886

Rating: 5 / 5 (80 voted)

Reviews: 95% of readers found this page helpful

Author information

Name: Clemencia Bogisich Ret

Birthday: 2001-07-17

Address: Suite 794 53887 Geri Spring, West Cristentown, KY 54855

Phone: +5934435460663

Job: Central Hospitality Director

Hobby: Yoga, Electronics, Rafting, Lockpicking, Inline skating, Puzzles, scrapbook

Introduction: My name is Clemencia Bogisich Ret, I am a super, outstanding, graceful, friendly, vast, comfortable, agreeable person who loves writing and wants to share my knowledge and understanding with you.